Naps Crucial For Brain Development and Memory in Kids


Summary: A new study aims to examine the role of napping in brain development among infants and preschoolers. By tracking changes in the hippocampus, the research aims to prove how critical naps are for memory retention and brain growth in young children.

These longitudinal studies could set new standards for nap policies in educational settings, benefiting both neurotypical and neurodiverse children. Insights from this research will provide valuable guidelines for parents and educators on the importance of napping in early childhood.

Key Facts:

  1. Focus on the Hippocampus: The studies investigate the hippocampus’s role during nap transitions, highlighting its significance in short-term memory and overall brain development.
  2. Longitudinal Approach: Unlike previous cross-sectional studies, these projects will observe the same children over time to better understand the developmental milestones associated with napping.
  3. Practical Applications: Findings from the studies are expected to influence nap policies in preschool environments and offer actionable insights for parents of young children.

Source: UMass

A University of Massachusetts Amherst sleep scientist, funded with $6.7 million in grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH), has launched two unprecedented studies that will track over time the brain development of infants and preschoolers to confirm the role of napping in early life and to identify the bioregulatory mechanisms involved.

Rebecca Spencer, a professor of psychological and brain sciences who is well-known for her groundbreaking research into napping, is testing her theories about what’s happening in the hippocampus–the short-term memory area of the brain–as babies and young children undergo nap transitions.

This shows a toddler napping.
Naps allow children with an immature hippocampus to process memories. Credit: Neuroscience News

This new research is expected to become the gold standard of scientific evidence that emphasizes the importance of healthy sleep for young children as their brains develop. 

The findings will help inform nap policies for preschool and pre-kindergarten and be useful to teachers and parents of both neurotypical and neurodiverse children.

“The work we’ve been doing has always pointed to this interaction of sleep and brain development,” says Spencer, who carries out research in her Somneurolab at UMass Amherst.

“We think that kids get ready to transition out of naps when the brain is big enough to hold all the information of the day until night-time sleep.”

The study involving preschoolers is a collaboration between Spencer at UMass Amherst; Tracy Riggins, a developmental psychologist specializing in memory development at the University of Maryland; and Gregory Hancock, a UMD professor of human development and quantitative methodology. 

Previous research by Spencer and Riggins showed differences in the hippocampus of kids who nap compared to those who have transitioned out of naps.

“So far, we’ve used cross-sectional approaches,” says Spencer, referring to research that analyzes data at one point in time, as opposed to longitudinal studies that involve repeated observation over time.

“We really need to show longitudinally within a child that the point when they transition out of naps is predicted by a transition in the development of their hippocampus.” 

The hippocampus is the short-term location for memories before they move to the cortex for long-term storage. Naps allow children with an immature hippocampus to process memories.

Young children give up their afternoon nap, not based on their age, but their brain development, Spencer hypothesizes.

“Naps are beneficial to everybody. Naps protect memory for everybody, no matter what age. Kids who are habitual nappers really need the nap. If they don’t nap, they get catastrophic forgetting.

“That’s the difference between habitual and non-habitual nappers – not how good is the nap, but how bad is staying awake,” Spencer explains.

Adds Riggins, “In the end, being able to tell parents that those little deviations from routine that keep their children from napping might not have these huge implications for a neurotypical child in the long run would be great.

“And, the more we know about how the brain works in a typically developing child during this nap transition, the more we will be able to know about where we could possibly intervene to help neurodiverse children–like children with autism and ADHD, whose sleep patterns tend to be disrupted–since we will have some sort of scientific basis.”

The research team is recruiting 180 children, ages 3 to 5 years. The researchers will track their brain development, memory performance and nap status over the course of one year at three checkpoints.

During the first and second sessions, the children will wear activity-tracking watches and EEG equipment to record naps and overnight sleep. They will also play memory games before and after naps. The children will undergo an MRI brain scan during the third session. 

Monica and David Dumlao, of Chicopee, Mass., signed up their son Miles, 4, for the preschool study after watching the Netflix documentary series, “Babies,” which featured Spencer in the episode about sleep.

“We like learning about the neuroscience behind brain development,” Monica Dumlao said at a recent study session in Spencer’s lab.

“We thought this was a good opportunity to contribute to the science about the importance of naps.” 

In the three-part infant study on nap transitions and memory, Spencer is studying the period before and after babies transition from two naps–one in the morning and one in the afternoon–to one, richer afternoon nap.

She is recruiting 140 infants 7 to 9 months old. The babies will play a memory game before and after their naps. Their brain activity will be recorded during their naps using a noninvasive electrode cap. The sessions will take place at 9, 12 and 15 months.

“We think as they are getting ready to drop the morning nap, staying awake in that morning interval will be less and less damaging to their memory,” Spencer says.

“But we don’t think that’s going to happen with the afternoon nap at this age. We think the afternoon nap stays superimportant.”

Microsleeps: The naps that may only last seconds.


Young woman sleeping in back of car (Credit: Getty Images)

Far shorter than a nap, microsleeps can last only a few seconds. They may be a sign of deeper problems getting decent sleep.

We’re not talking about a five-minute doze on the sofa while you’re bingeing on a box set. That counts as a long time in the world of microsleeping. Here, naps are measured in seconds.

While there’s no single definition scientists use, many studies focus on shuteye that lasts from one to 15 seconds at a time. And there is a growing understanding of how microsleeps affect our daily lives, from intruding into mundane everyday tasks to putting lives at risk in certain situations, such as driving a car. But we are also getting closer to knowing why they might happen in the first place.

Recent research has revealed that the real experts in these tiny sleeps are chinstrap penguins. While their mates are at sea foraging for food, the penguins left behind nest en masse and must stay alert to protect their eggs from predators like the brown skua, as well as from aggression from other penguins.  

Paul-Antoine Libourel, a sleep ecophysiologist at the Neuroscience Research Center of Lyon in France, measured the brain activity and sleep patterns of 14 penguins on King George Island in Antarctica. Over a 10-day period, the penguins never slept for longer than 34 seconds at a time. Instead they had more than 10,000 microsleeps lasting less than four seconds, sometimes in just one half of the brain at a time.

Four seconds might seem too short to have any restorative role, but the time adds up and each penguin was getting an impressive 11 hours of a sleep every 24 hours.

It seems to work for them, but for the modern human, microsleeps don’t get such a good press. The life-or-death situation where we need to stay alert, more often involves driving a big box of metal along a road at high speeds than avoiding a predator. Unfortunately, it’s a task that can be monotonous, but one that requires constant attention.If you pull an all-night shift, the tendency to nod off the next day will be hard to resist (Credit: Getty Images)

If you pull an all-night shift, the tendency to nod off the next day will be hard to resist

This is just the kind of situation (but without the risks) that experimenters have sought to replicate in the laboratory in order to investigate the nature of microsleeps.

In 2014, the first study exploring microsleeps by simultaneously using brain scanners, video recordings of people’s eyes and electroencephalogram (EEG) equipment to measure brainwaves was published. The team devised a very dull task for their unlucky participants. While lying in the scanner, they had a screen in front of them and a joystick in one hand. Their job was to use the joystick to ensure that a disc on the screen constantly kept up with a moving target. 

It was so boring that people struggled to stay awake – 70% had at least 36 microsleeps during the 50-minute session. The scientists, based in Christchurch, New Zealand, expected a bit of napping, but not this much. Yes, the people had just had lunch and were lying down, but still, they weren’t sleep-deprived and anyone who’s been inside a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner will know that there’s a constant backdrop of a banging noise. It wouldn’t seem like the ideal place to nod off.

The more fatigued we are, the more likely we are to microsleep

Not surprisingly, microsleeps are even more common in people with narcolepsy. But research suggests that most of us are having them.

By giving people a fake steering wheel and a track to follow, other researchers at the University of Canterbury in Christchurch, observed them becoming drowsy with boredom, and demonstrating that all too familiar sequence you might know from trying to stay awake during a lecture in a warm room, where your eyelids close for a moment, your head nods a little and then suddenly jerks as you wake up again. 

The more fatigued we are, the more likely we are to microsleep. Yvonne Harrison, a sleep researcher at the University of Loughborough, UK, found that these naps are more common in the afternoons and evenings and often precede a lengthier period of sleep

David Dinges, a professor of psychiatry at the University of Pennsylvania, keeps people awake all night to see how often they doze off the next day. After pulling an all-nighter, as you might expect, their lapses in attention (considered by many scientists to be microsleeps), become much more frequent. But, alarmingly in terms of road safety, he found that when people got six hours sleep a night for 14 days in a row, they had just as many microsleeps as those who had missed a whole night’s sleep.Chinstrap penguins may nap up to 10,000 times in a day - but humans can't get the sleep they need in the same way (Credit: Getty Images)

Chinstrap penguins may nap up to 10,000 times in a day – but humans can’t get the sleep they need in the same way.

But unlike them, the people getting six hours’ sleep a night didn’t realise they were so fatigued. They just got on with it.

And we don’t even always know we’ve had a microsleep. In one study people were invited to deliberately take daytime naps in the lab and were then woken by the researchers after either a minute, five minutes, 10 minutes or 20 and asked whether they had been asleep or not. If they had been allowed to sleep for just 60 seconds, only 15% realised they had napped at all. Even after a 10-minute sleep, only half acknowledged they had fallen asleep.

This might make sense of those times where people strenuously deny to you that they nodded off in front of the TV, even though you could see they were clearly fast asleep

In terms of what is happening in the brain, microsleeps are largely characterised by a shift from alpha to theta waves, the kind of waves you see in the first stage of sleep. So far, so similar to normal sleep. But something else curious might be going on.

The New Zealand experiment where people followed a target with a joystick revealed an intriguing finding that surprised the researchers. Rather than a consistent winding down of activity in the brain as people nodded off, while some areas did become less active as expected, they saw something else: an increase in activity in the frontal and parietal cortex, the areas stretching back behind our foreheads.

A two-second sleep is enough for a car or a lorry to swerve into the next lane, which of course can be fatal

The team speculates that this might be an unconscious response to drifting off, an attempt not to sleep, but to stay awake. This isn’t seen in longer sleeps or napper, suggesting that microsleeps are different from normal sleep and not just shorter.

Analysing the data from this experiment in more detail nine years later, the team found not a decrease, but an increase in delta, beta and gamma waves during a microsleep, backing up the idea that when we fall asleep, but should be concentrating, our brains are aware of this and work to initiate a process to wake us up again.

When you’re not driving these sleeps might not matter that much, risking maybe missing out on a few seconds of a film or play, or at worse embarrassment if you are caught getting a bit of shuteye while someone is talking to you.

The problem is of course that with modern, fast vehicles, a two-second sleep is enough for a car or a lorry to swerve into the next lane, which of course can be fatal. So, we can’t risk any unplanned naps while we’re driving, however momentary they might be. Recent research conducted within a single truck company in Japan, employing almost 15,000 drivers, is instructive. Scientists analysed the dashcam footage from 52 of their professional drivers who had been involved in collisions after falling asleep.A microsleep of a few seconds can be very dangerous – even potentially fatal – if you're at the wheel of car (Credit: Getty Images)

A microsleep of a few seconds can be very dangerous – even potentially fatal – if you’re at the wheel of car.

The videos revealed that three quarters of them showed signs of a microsleep before the collision. The sequences of events for one driver went like this. A minute before the crash he started blinking rapidly, 38 seconds before the crash his body stopped moving, followed by some slow blinking and then four seconds before the accident his eyes closed for just two seconds. They opened again one second before the crash, but it was too late for him to do anything and the truck had left the road.

From these videos the researchers have identified ways of predicting that a crash would soon happen, which could be used to alert the driver. When they feel tired drivers try to stay awake by touching their face or body (they don’t give details, but I’m guessing this means something similar to slapping our own faces in an effort to wake ourselves up). They might stretch or fidget before the microsleep takes place. The researchers suggesting that any automatic system to detect that a driver is about to nod off includes much more than just whether they eyes close, but looks at wider movements in the body.

In the meantime as drivers, we need to be aware that we’re feeling sleepy, which is harder than it sounds considering that we don’t always detect that we’ve nodded off for a second. And if we do know we’re exhausted, just trying hard to concentrate isn’t always enough.

Willpower is not enough even when you know it will keep you alive

This was evidenced by the deaths among pilots returning to England after bombing raids during the Second World War, sleep research Jim Horne wrote in his book Sleepfaring: a Journey Through the Science of Sleep. During the dogfights their fear, focus, efforts to win, and adrenaline kept them awake, however tired they were. But once there were no distractions on the way home, there were some pilots who were overcome with the need to sleep and crashed their planes as a result.

Willpower is not enough even when you know it will keep you alive, on a long drive we need to heed the advice to pull over in a safe place as soon as you feel tired, to drink coffee, have a nap, and then wait for a time until you are fully awake before continuing driving.

For humans, unlike chinstrap penguins, microsleeps are no substitute for a proper night’s sleep. If they are very frequent, then it might suggest we’re not getting enough sleep. But when we’re not driving or doing something else where concentration is vital, perhaps we shouldn’t worry about a few seconds of extra shuteye here and there.

Sleeping boosts women’s brain power but men benefit more from naps, research suggests


Previous research has suggested women need more sleep as their brains are ‘more complex’ than men’s.

Getting a good night’s sleep boosts women’s brain power, while men benefit from shorter naps, research has suggested.

sleep-rex.jpg

Scientists at the Max Planck Institute in Munich analysed the sleep patterns of 160 adults to consider how sleep affects intellectual capacity. Their study has been presented at the Forum of Neuroscience in Copenhagen.

The researchers monitored the cohort’s sleep patterns, as well as performing intelligence tests on them to assess their reasoning and problem solving skills, Mail Online reports. They monitored sleep spindles, which are bursts of brain activity to consider correlation with different forms of sleep, cross-referenced with gender.

They found that sleep spindles, which are associated with higher IQ scores, were boosted when women entered dreamless sleep. For men no such correlation was found during dreamless sleep.

However, analysis of men’s brain activity found the same stimulation occurred when they had naps.

Professor Martin Dresler said: “Our results demonstrate that the association between sleep spindles and intelligence is more complex than we have assumed until now.

“There are many factors involved in intellectual abilities, and sleep is just one of them. This large study of men and women gives us a more accurate framework for the next phase of research which will involve differences in individuals sleep patterns.”

Earlier this year, researchers at Loughborough University Sleep Research Centre found women may need more sleep as their brains are more complex than men’s. Professor Jim Horne, who has researched the issue said: “Women tend to multi-task — they do lots at once and are flexible — and so they use more of their actual brain than men do. Because of that, their sleep need is greater.”