HUMANS IN DIFFERENT BUILDINGS LINKED BRAIN-TO-BRAIN


INTERFACE LETS A PERSON READ ANOTHER’S THOUGHTS

University of Washington graduate student Jose Ceballos wears an electroencephalography (EEG) cap that records brain activity and sends a response to a second participant over the Internet.

In one lab, an image of an object is shown to a study participant on a computer screen. The subject wears a cap connected to an electroencephalography (EEG) machine that monitors his brain waves.

Another participant, the asker, sits in another lab on campus. It’s her job to figure out what image the answerer was shown. The asker has a magnetic coil almost touching her skull. She selects a question from a list on a touchscreen to narrow down the object, 20-questions style.

The question she asks appears on the screen in front of the answerer. To the left and right of the screen are two LED lights, flashing at different frequencies. One side is labeled ‘yes’ and the other ‘no’. He answers the question by staring at the correct light. Chantel Prat, a psychologist at the University of Washington and a co-author on the paper, told Popular Sciencethat the computer can distinguish what light he looks at because different frequencies of light trigger different brain patterns in response.

The computer algorithm sends its interpretation of the answerer’s brain patterns to the magnetic coil in the asker’s lab over the Internet. If the answer is yes, the coil releases a pulse that stimulates the asker’s visual cortex in the brain. The stimulation triggers a phosphene, which can look like a flash of light, a blob, a blur, or a wavy line. If it’s no, the coil still makes the same sounds and scalp-tingling sensations, but doesn’t release a pulse strong enough to cause a phosphene.

The asker notes whether they saw the phosphene flash or not, and moves onto the next question. It’s a game of 20 Questions played brain to brain.

University of Washington postdoctoral student Caitlin Hudac wears a cap that uses transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMG) to deliver brain signals from the other participant.

In the study, published Wednesday in PLOS One, participants guessed the correct object 72 percent of the time. To make sure it wasn’t due to chance or cheating, the researchers also ran control games where the participants thought they were communicating but no data was being sent. In those conditions, they only guessed correctly 18 percent of the time.

This is a big step forward from the group’s 2014 experiment, in which they successfully transmitted the brain pattern of hand movement to another person’s brain, causing the second person’s hand to move. In this study, the asker consciously acknowledges incoming information from other person’s brain and makes a decision based on it. It’s real communication.

There are limitations. The answerer isn’t just thinking about the word ‘yes,’ the way most of us would imagine mind-to-mind communication, but staring at a light that has been assigned to mean ‘yes.’ And right now, the asker can’t respond brain-to-brain, though that’s something the team is working on. The team is also experimenting with ways to move beyond binary yes/no type answers. Prat said they could use different kinds of stimulations to allow more answer options. They could use quadrants of space or different visual or auditory cues and patterns.

But these are all still symbolic patterns of light or sensation that are translated into meaning. Our spoken language is actually no different. Words and letters still only symbolize intent or emotion, and often fail to capture what we really mean. This type of brain linking could one day change that.

Prat said, “Imagine if you could just transmit the feeling of experiencing something sweet.” She said that already scientists are able to identify what object a person is thinking of, given a list of possibilities. The difficulty is recreating those abstract brain signals in another person, especially when the subject becomes more complicated than an object.

If all this has you reaching for a tin hat, don’t worry. “People get nervous that others could read their thoughts without them being aware,” said Prat. “It’s not possible that someone could be doing this to you without you knowing about it and consenting to it.” The equipment is expensive, bulky and very finicky. Moving the magnetic coil even a quarter inch away from the asker’s skull nullified the effect, and both participants had to be actively focusing on communicating.

The next step for Prat’s team is to test more immediate, practical applications for the technology, like yoking the brains of high-achieving students with those of struggling students. They want to see if they can boost learning by signaling the brain that it is time to pay attention. “Hopefully we will run that experiment in the next six months,” she said.

The Difference Between the Somogyi Effect and Dawn Phenomenon


The somogyi effect (first discovered my Dr. Michael Somogyi) is caused by nighttime hypoglycemia, which leads to a rebound hyperglycemia in the early morning hours.  You see, when your blood glucose drops during the night, hormones are released which trigger the liver to release stored glucose. This normally results in a high-fasting glucose reading the next morning.

The Somogyi effect is a result of having extra insulin the body before bedtime, either from not having a bedtime snack, or from having your long-acting insulin not at the proper dose.  The Somogyi effect occurs mainly with type 1 diabetics.

It is similar to the dawn phenomenon in that both lead to high morning blood glucose readings as a result of a hormone release that causes the liver to release glucose into the blood. The difference is that dawn phenomenon is not caused by hypoglycemia, but by a random release of the triggering hormones.

How To Treat The Dawn Phenomenon?

One of the very first (and obvious) steps that you should take is to contact your doctors office.  Once you and your doctor figure out how your blood sugar levels behave overnight, here are some suggestions that he or she may start to incorporate:

  • Take long-acting insulin in the evening so its peak action happens when your blood sugars start rising.
  • Change the type of insulin you take in the evening.
  • Start incorporating a continuous glucose monitor (CGM) to avoid the dawn phenomenon
  • Take a small amount of insulin overnight if your blood sugar goes up during the night.
  • Switch to an insulin pump, which can be programmed to automatically increase your basal rates in the morning.  This works perfectly for me with my omni pod pump system from Insulet.

GIANT SOLAR “TULIPS” WILL HELP ETHIOPIA BECOME CARBON NEUTRAL BY 2025


 

Around the globe, more countries are looking to reduce their carbon emissions by bolstering renewable energy infrastructure, but they can’t do it alone. Nations with eco-friendly initiatives rely on energy and technology companies to help them meet their goals. In Ethiopia, a partnership with AORA Solar, developer of solar-biogas hybrid power technology, may help that country reach its goal of becoming carbon neutral by 2025. AORA will join forces with three universities in Ethiopia to promote academic cooperation for the development and advancement of renewable energy technologies in the middle-income country.
AORA-Solar-partnering-with-Ethiopian-universities-537x403AORA-Solar-MoU-with-Ethiopian-universities-537x358

The partnership is in the form of a Memorandum of Understanding (MoU), which AORA Solar announced they signed this week with Arizona State University LightWorks, Addis Ababa Science and Technology University (AASTU) and Adama Science and Technology University (ASTU). Under the MoU, educational and research programs will be launched to support the national energy goals, and the efforts will be overseen by the Ethiopian Ministry of Water, Irrigation and Energy and the Ethiopian Ministry of Science and Technology.

In addition to renewable energy research, AORA Solar’s involvement with the universities will lead to the installation of AORA’s solar-biogas hybrid power technology at both Ethiopian institutions. “Our ongoing engagement with AORA Solar illustrates our commitment to conducting use-inspired research, engaging globally, valuing entrepreneurship and providing students with the necessary skills and knowledge to affect a transition to a sustainable world,” said Gary Dirks director of LightWorks and director of the Julie Ann Wrigley Global Institute of Sustainability at ASU. They went on to say the partnership could enhance the lives of Ethiopian citizens. Surely, if the 2025 carbon neutral goal is met in part due to this relationship, that will be true.

MORE PEOPLE HAVE DIED THIS YEAR FROM SELFIES THAN SHARKS


30814b292f75ea0b00b8beea62760b37

The media hype over shark attacks reached insane levels this year. This led conservationists like Richard BransonShark Attack Survivors for Shark Conservation and The Pew Charitable Trusts to support efforts to restore and conserve the world’s shark populations—warning against turning an uptick in attacks into “an excuse to kill more sharks.” After all, humans kill far more sharks every year—at least 100 million more—than sharks kill people.

And get this: According to Mashable, more people have died from selfies than shark attacks this year. Earlier this month, a park in Colorado had to close because too many people were getting dangerously close to bears to take selfies. And this was no isolated incident. Wildlife authorities in the Lake Tahoe area, Yellowstone National Park and other recreation areas have repeatedly cautioned visitors against getting too close to bears, bison and other large animals.

CMzVifJUEAE_g0u

CI8qWQpVAAAY1MU

The latest death by selfie happened when a 66-year-old Japanese tourist died, and his travel companion was injured, after falling down stairs while attempting to take a selfie at the Taj Mahal.

“The man’s death raises the selfie toll this year to 12,” says Mashable. “To put that in perspective, in 2015 there have so far been eight deaths caused by shark attacks.”

This seems like a joke right? Sadly, it’s not. “Other deaths have been caused by distracted photo-takers falling off cliffs, crashing their cars, being hit by trains, and even shooting themselves while posing with guns,” said Science Alert. “It’s not clear if the number of daredevil selfies is increasing, but more and more tourists are making headlines because of their dangerous attempts at a memorable photo,” says Mashable.

twitter @zodoyle Nearly died talking #TDFselfie

This has left many prominent places no choice but to ban selfies or—at least selfie sticks in the case of Disney. Pamplona banned selfies at its famed running of the bulls last year. The Tour de France is concerned about selfies endangering people, too.

twitter @tatsumi38

Australia has issues with dare devils taking selfies too. They can’t get people to stop taking pictures on this rock that is on the verge of crumbling into the ocean.

And it’s been such a problem in Russia that the government launched a campaign to warn people about the dangers of selfies.

Photo credit: Russian Ministry of the Interior

Genetic clue to breast cancer relapses


 

Secondary or metastatic breast cancer

Scientists say they have discovered a genetic clue to why some breast cancers relapse, which could lead to better treatment.

A research team from the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute in Cambridge found that cancers that return were more likely to contain certain genes or combinations of genes.

Targeting these genes with early treatments could be key, they said.

The study is being presented at the European Cancer Congress in Vienna.

In around one in five people with breast cancer, the disease will return – either to the same place as the original tumour or another part of the body.

Dr Lucy Yates, clinical research oncologist from the Sanger Institute in Cambridge, and her colleagues analysed data from the tumours of 1,000 breast cancer patients including 161 people whose cancer had recurred or spread.

When comparing primary and secondary tumours, they found noticeable genetic differences and several of the mutations present in the secondary cancers were relatively uncommon in cancers diagnosed for the first time.

Dr Yates said the patterns they found suggested that the complement of cancer genes in some primary cancers may make them more likely to relapse in the future, while additional cancer genes acquired after diagnosis may drive the cancer relapse.

She said doctors might be able to use this knowledge to identify patients at high risk of their cancer returning and pick the best treatment for targeting particular genetic mutations.

This would mean taking regular samples of cancer tissue to track how the disease is progressing and changing.

Dr Yates said: “Further work is needed to validate these findings in much larger datasets, but we hope that in the future it will be possible at the point of diagnosis to look at the cancer genes in an individual’s cancer and determine whether it is likely to return in the future and, if so, to select a personalised therapy to prevent that event.”

Prof Peter Naredi, scientific co-chair of the congress, said the results of the research were “very important in the era of precision medicine”.

He said: “This study underlines the fact that we should consider a recurrence of a cancer as a new event and carefully select the right treatment for the recurrent tumour as opposed to just relying on information from the first occurrence.”

Dr Jorge Reis-Filho, from the Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Centre in New York, said the study demonstrated “the importance of analysing the genetic features of metastases when making treatment decisions”.